Philosophical Perspectives
INTRODUCTION
Theoretical perspectives such as behaviorism, cognitivism,
cognitive constructivism, sociocultural/historicism,
and situativity theory provide frameworks for
describing learning and designing instruction. Finding
roots in philosophy, these perspectives differ with
respect to their ontological and epistemological
assumptions. Learning theories and instructional theories
are developed and linked to a particular set of
assumptions, supposedly consistent with one of the
theoretical perspectives. Duffy and Jonassen (1992)
argued that instructional strategies and methods are
clearly influenced by the philosophical assumptions
and that theories of knowing and learning are implicit
in the instructional design. If not implemented entirely
by a cookbook approach, then when situational variables
require some decision on the part of the educator
an underlying set of assumptions (whether they be tacit
or explicit) will drive the decision (Barab and Duffy,
2000). It is inconceivable that a teacher or instructional
designer would advocate a particular lesson or activity
without at least a tacit theory of how students think
and learn.
In the literature, we see various classifications for
these different perspectives; for example, Greeno et al.
(1996) described behaviorist/empiricist, cognitivist/
rationalist, and situative/pragmatist-sociohistorical
perspectives. Prawat and Floden (1994) used worldviews
to define their classifications: mechanistic
(including information processing approaches), organismic
(including radical constructivism), and contextualist
(including social constructivism). Wood (1995)
grounded his categories in the application of learning
theory to technology: Skinner and neo-behaviorism,
Piaget and constructivist theory, Vygotsky and social
constructivism and situated cognition.
Unfortunately, when considering theoretical perspectives
and the learning theories that have developed
within them, it is not always clear what the underlying
philosophical roots are. In fact, it becomes confusing
when considering the descriptions from authors distinguishing
differently among theoretical perspectives.
Driscoll (1994), for example, stated that Piaget’s developmental
theory and constructivism were interpretivist
based. Cobb (1994), when distinguishing among the
cognitive and socioconstructivists, aligned the cognitive
constructivist with the views of von Glasersfeld
(1989), who used Piaget as his example; yet, von Glasersfeld
described the basis as pragmatist. Greeno and
colleagues (1996), Cobb (1994), and Driscoll (1994)
placed the socioconstructivist or sociohistorical perspective
under the roots of pragmatism as well, calling
on the views of Vygotsky. Phillips (1995) distinguished among the various sects of constructivism,
placing the perspectives by Piaget and Vygotsky
together based on the unit of analysis. Greeno and
colleagues (1996) classified constructivism along with
cognitivism, finding roots in rationalism. Ertmer and
Newby (1993) located cognitivism and behaviorism
within the objectivist perspective, with cognitivism as
well as constructivism being rationalism.
To further confound things, Garrison (1995)
equated contemporary social constructivism with what
he defined as pragmatic social behaviorism based on
the work of Dewey, indicating a relationship between
constructivism and adaptations of behaviorism. Garrison
also stated that situated cognition (classified within
the situative/pragmatist-sociohistoric category by
Greeno et al., 1996) has made an important contribution
to social constructivism. Greeno (1998) stated that the
situative perspective could subsume both the behaviorist
and the cognitivist perspectives. Whereas Prawat and
Floden (1994) combined social constructivism and situativity
perspectives, Derry (1992) distinguished
between constructivist and culturally situated learning
views. Among situated perspectives, Lave (1997) further
divided what she termed cognition plus, interpretivist
views, and her situated social practice view.
What factors are being used to distinguish the
above theoretical perspectives? Epistemological or
ontological assumptions? For seasoned theorists in the
field, these distinctions may be trivial to sort out, or
maybe some believe that these distinctions have little
practical significance. For those beginning their scholarship
in the field, confusion seems to reign. Further,
for those interested in designing practical applications
of instruction and seeking the grounding that a theoretical
foundation can provide, it may be difficult to
understand the foundation on which they are building.
It is in response to these questions, and with the
goal of providing sharper boundaries among these categories
to inform learning architects, that we have
written this chapter. We describe theoretical perspectives
as subsumed under five categories and clarify
these categories by defining the mind/body relations,
epistemology, ontology, and the unit of analysis. Then,
we turn to the instructional implications of these five
categories. It is important to recognize that these distinctions
are situated within the context of providing
sharper boundaries and stimulating discussion. As
such, we have drawn lines among perspectives that
may seem overly defined and may not even exist within
other contexts and for other purposes; for example,
some theorists would not separate situativity and constructivist
perspectives or would not build connections
between objectivism and cognitivism. In writing this
paper, therefore, we have made epistemological and ontological commitments—a process that we suggest
is useful for instructional designers and educational
psychologists to undertake.
This is not to imply that we have simply constructed
these distinctions based on our fancy; in fact, we have
aligned each conjecture with citations from colleagues
in the field, stacking our allies if you will (Latour,
1987). Our hesitancy in forwarding these categories is
that readers will take these brief and overly simplified
categories as fixed and rigid rules or, even worse, as
substitutes for involved study of the particular philosophical
works discussed. It is our intention that these
categories should not be used for compartmentalizing
but should serve as a backdrop for continued discussion
and for broader discourse among our colleagues.
No comments:
Post a Comment